Normal people working in normal organizations with normal equipment:
System safety and cognition in a mid-air collision
Paulo Victor Rodrigues de Carvalhoa,b,*, Jose´ Orlando Gomesb, Gilbert Jacob Huberb, Mario Cesar Vidalc
a Comissa˜o Nacional de Energia Nuclear, Instituto de Engenharia Nuclear, Cidade Univerisitária, Ilha do Funda˜o, CEP 21945-970 Rio de Janeiro, RJ, Brazil
b Graduate Program in Informatics-NCE&IM, Cidade Universitária, Rio de Janeiro, RJ, Brazil
c Luis Alberto Coimbra Research Institute, COPPE, Federal University of Rio de Janeiro, Rio de Janeiro, RJ, Brazil
a r t i c l e i n f o
Article history:
Received 1 February 2008
Accepted 28 November 2008
Keywords:
Mid-air collision
System safety
Cognitive strategies
Air traffic management system
a b s t r a c t
A fundamental challenge in improving the safety of complex systems is to understand how accidents
emerge in normal working situations, with equipment functioning normally in normally structured
organizations. We present a field study of the en route mid-air collision between a commercial carrier
and an executive jet, in the clear afternoon Amazon sky in which 154 people lost their lives, that illus-
trates one response to this challenge. Our focus was on how and why the several safety barriers of a well
structured air traffic system melted down enabling the occurrence of this tragedy, without any cata-
strophic component failure, and in a situation where everything was functioning normally. We identify
strong consistencies and feedbacks regarding factors of system day-to-day functioning that made
monitoring and awareness difficult, and the cognitive strategies that operators have developed to deal
with overall system behavior. These findings emphasize the active problem-solving behavior needed in
air traffic control work, and highlight how the day-to-day functioning of the system can jeopardize such
behavior. An immediate consequence is that safety managers and engineers should review their tradi-
tional safety approach and accident models based on equipment failure probability, linear combinations
of failures, rules and procedures, and human errors, to deal with complex patterns of coincidence
possibilities, unexpected links, resonance among system functions and activities, and system cognition.
© 2008 Elsevier Ltd. All rights reserved.
.if there is no seed, if the bramble of cause, agency, and
procedure does not issue from a fault nucleus, but is rather
unstably perched between scales, between human and non-
human, and between protocol and judgment, then the world is
a more disordered and dangerous place
Galison (2000), p. 32
1. Introduction
Mid-air collisions of en route aircraft are extremely rare events. A
review of air traffic management (ATM) related accidents world-
wide, from 1980 to 2001, (Van Es, 2003) showed that ATM-related
accidents account for 8% of all accidents (the ATM-related accident
rate is 0.44 per million flights). This review also showed that most
the fatalities are caused by mid-air collisions (63%), and the major
causal factors (classified according to the ICAO taxonomy – Flight
Crew, Air Traffic Controller (ATC), Environmental, and Aircraft
System) that contributed to the mid-air collisions were: 1) ATC –
Failure to provide separation – air, and 2) Flight Crew – Lack of
positional awareness – in air.
In this paper, we use a systemic framework to analyze the
functioning of the ATM cognitive system during the mid-air
collision between flight GLO1907 (a commercial aircraft Boeing
737-800) and flight N600XL (an EMBRAER E-145 Legacy jet) to
understand how and why this tragedy happened. This ATM-related
accident occurred at 16:56 Brazilian time on September 29, 2006, in
the clear afternoon Amazon sky. Our aim is to understand how
a mid-air collision can still happen, despite the various defense
layers that exist in the ATM system to prevent just such an event.
Based on our findings we develop some insights about the cognitive
functioning of the Brazilian Air Traffic Controllers and its safety
implications to the ATM system operation.
Accidents and incidents by themselves cannot be considered
absolute and direct indicators of the safety of any system (Woods
and Cook, 2006). However, the analysis of the dynamic interplay of
loosely and tightly coupled subsystems during the emergence of
incidents and accidents can reveal patterns of behavior in the
* Corresponding author. Comissa˜o Nacional de Energia Nuclear, Instituto de
Engenharia Nuclear, Cidade Univerisitária, Ilha do Funda˜o, CEP 21945-970 Rio de
Janeiro, RJ, Brazil. Tel.: þ55 21 21733835.
E-mail address: [email protected] (P.V. Rodrigues de Carvalho).
Contents lists available at ScienceDirect
Applied Ergonomics
journal homepage: www.elsevier.com/locate/apergo
0003-6870/$ – see front matter © 2008 Elsevier Ltd. All rights reserved.
doi:10.1016/j.apergo.2008.11.013
Applied Ergonomics 40 (2009) 325–340

functioning of the whole system that may indicate a drift to unsafe
states (Snook, 2000). The basic safety goal of the ATM system
during en route commercial flights is to avoid mid-air collisions.
Therefore, an analysis of the system operation during a mid-air
collision can provide insights about system weaknesses and safety.
1.1. A systemic framework for accident analysis
Modern organizations are complex sociotechnical systems
comprised of many nested levels: government, regulators,
company, management, staff, and activities/work/processes/tech-
nical systems. According to Rasmussen and Svedung (2000), safety
can be viewed as a control problem involving all these levels and
must be managed by a control structure embedded in an adaptive
sociotechnical system. From this perspective, accidents are emer-
gent properties of complex systems that are more prone to occur
when the control systems (safety barriers included) do not
adequately handle the day-to-day system failures/disturbances in
a broad sense (external disturbances, component failures, human
failures, or dysfunctional interactions among system components),
throughout the system’s life cycle (Hollnagel, 2004). The outcome
of a controlled system – the result of reasonably foreseen input
changes – is directly influenced by the system’s control mecha-
nisms in place. Hazardous situations occur when flaws in the
control mechanisms (technical, human, organizational) enable the
emergence of unexpected outcomes that generate negative
consequences. Accidents in safety-critical systems, like ATM, with
many layers of control (defense-in-depth concept) can happen only
when there are simultaneous failures in various control mecha-
nisms in different system layers (Hollnagel, 2006).
ATM systems are composed of many nested layers resulting in
complex interactions. Interactions occur between human operators
(controllers and pilots), between human operators and procedures
(flight plans, rules to define the controlled air space, the air space
sectors that must be handled by some specific controller team,
general safety rules for the control of traffic, and so forth), and
between operators and hardware/software technical systems
(radar systems, computer processing of radar and flight data,
aircraft navigation systems, traffic alert and collision avoidance
system – TCAS, communication systems between controllers and
pilots, flight progress strips).
Brooker (2006) simplified the ATM system description into
three structural system layers acting as the system controls: Plan-
ning (pre-operational), Operation (the flight in progress), and Alert
(the ground and air protection enabled by conflict alert systems, on
which the controller/pilot will act). Humans in the several ATM
system layers participate in control of the system, acting to operate
the system in a safe manner. Failure of system control occurs when
the mechanisms used to keep system stability fail and make the
situation worse against reasonably foreseen threats. The funda-
mental issue here is how to identify reasonably foreseen threats in
dynamic real-life situations. To do so, we must understand how
people address the overall constraints of the control system during
their daily work. The most important question regarding human
control performance in safety-critical systems is to understand how
and why normal work done by normal people enables the emer-
gence of accidents (Dekker, 2006).
2. Method
The research methodology to investigate the functioning and
safety of the ATM system based on the analysis of a single case
study – the GLO1907/N600XL collision – can be justified by Yin’s
(1994) rationale. According to Yin case studies can be used when
how and why questions are being posed, and when the focus is on
a contemporary phenomenon within some real-life context.
Understanding HOW two airplanes collided in the clear afternoon
Amazon sky satisfies Yin’s HOW criterion. The concurrence of
organizational and cognitive factors that enable the emergence of
this tragedy – directly related to the ATM system’s safety – satisfies
Yin’s WHY criterion.
Catastrophic accidents in the domain of ultra-safe systems –
such as commercial fixed wing scheduled flight, with risk lower
than 10À6 – provide a unique opportunity to study the safety of
complex systems. This condition also justifies the use of a single
case study according to Yin’s rationale: an extreme and unique case
or a revelatory one (Yin, 1994). The mid-air collision satisfies these
two criteria. It is unique and extreme in the sense that mid-air
collisions are the least frequent ATM-related accidents, and it is
revelatory due to the generation of a considerable amount of data
that become available to the public and researchers through
secondary sources. The availability of these data is especially
important in the case of the Brazilian ATM system as it is operated
under military administration and the data about its functioning
(e.g. danger reports, near misses) are not usually available to the
public and social scientists.
In our research method, we search for the mid-air collision
antecedents traced through the concurrence of performances of the
several actors (pilots and controllers) and their interaction with the
contextual conditions of the several ATM subsystems. Our aim is to
understand how and why this accident happened based on the rich
data set that, because the occurrence of this tragedy is now available
to the public. Data and evidences, antecedents and consequences
from this mid-air collision come from a wide range of publicly
available sources. These sources include official government docu-
ments, congressional hearings, including controllers’, pilots’, and air
traffic management authorities’ testimonies, video tapes, audio
tapes of many media centers, press releases, newspaper clippings,
flight plans, regulations, maps, directives and so forth.
3. The air traffic management system – ATM
To understand the control functions of the ATM system, we use
the systems layers concepts as defined by Brooker (2006). The ATM
system layers are formed by human, technological (hardware and
software), and organizational components. Some of them, impor-
tant to avoid mid-air collisions, are briefly described:
– Controllers and pilots, sharp-end operators at the bottom layer
of the system;
– Prescribed safety rules for the control of traffic, including the
minimum separation to be permitted between aircraft, flight
plans;
– Communications equipment between controllers and pilots
(special radio communications frequencies);
– Airways to structure aircraft traffic in the controlled air space
and provide references for traffic separation;
– Air space sectors to allow the division of air traffic control
among different controller teams;
– Many other software rules to discipline where and how the
different types of aircraft must fly;
– Flight progress strips based on the flight plan data used by
controllers to quickly recall the order (in time) and the details
of the flights they have to handle; recommendations regarding
the maximum number of flights that can be managed by the
controller;
– Radar systems:
B Primary Surveillance Radar (PSR), that works by passively
bouncing a radio signal off the skin of the aircraft and whose
advantage is that it operates totally independently of the
P.V. Rodrigues de Carvalho et al. / Applied Ergonomics 40 (2009) 325–340
326

target aircraft – that is, no action from the aircraft is
required for it to provide a radar return – but that does not
positively identify the aircraft, generates imprecise altitude
information, and has a range limited by distance, altitude,
terrain and rain or snow;
B Secondary Surveillance Radar (SSR) that overcomes these
limitations but depends on a transponder in the aircraft to
respond to interrogations from the ground station to make
the plane more visible and to report the aircraft’s altitude.
– Computer processing complementing the information coming
from the radar and flight data;
– High quality aircraft navigation systems using Inertial Naviga-
tion Systems through to satellite-based aids such GPS;
– Short Term Conflict Alert (STCA) and Separation Monitoring
Function (SMF) that are the computer processing systems for
analyzing the radar tracks to predict if aircraft might come into
proximity soon and, if they might, warn the controller by
flashing a message on his radar screen;
– Traffic Alert and Collision Avoidance System (TCAS), the on
board collision avoidance systems based on detection of other
aircraft in the vicinity through their SSR transponders. These
inform the pilot of nearby traffic – TA (Traffic Advisory) – and
aircraft coming into conflict – RA (Resolution Advisory). RAs
inform the pilot to climb or descend as appropriate to take the
flight out of risk.
In an ATM control system functioning as described above, the
safety control barriers to avoid a mid-air collision can be summa-
rized as:
– Controlled air space with straight route, with two traffic lanes,
– Opposite traffic flows along each lane,
– Distance between the two lanes of 1000 ft (vertical separation),
– Well defined flight plans received before the flight,
– Traffic flow per lane lower than 4 aircraft/hour (longitudinal
separation),
– The two aircraft are equipped with Traffic Alert and Collision
Avoidance System (TCAS),
– The two aircraft are under surveillance of ground controllers,
with radar tracking and radio communication.
In a system with this entire set of control safety barriers, how
could an accident like this ever happen? How a could a sophisti-
cated ATM system with several safety barriers, many coordinating
mechanisms, surveillance
and
communication
equipment
providing redundant layers of cross-checking possibilities allow
this to happen? There was no emergency situation, no weather
problems in the clear afternoon Amazon sky, and no sudden
equipment failure that can be considered the cause of this tragedy.
3.1. The configuration of the Brazilian ATM system
Because the research approach attaches great significance to the
work environment as the root of variation in decision-making and
cognitive behavior, we present a brief description of the Brazilian
ATM system using the means-end hierarchy (Rasmussen et al.,
1994). In particular, we will use the Rasmussen and Svedung (2000)
framework for risk management to look at management and
organization structures. Fig. 1 presents the generic actor map for
the Brazilian ATM system. This diagram provides a view of the
whole system, including many levels ranging from governmental
structures at the top, down to the local environment of sharp-end
operators (controllers and pilots), the ultimate level related to the
collision. The lower level represents the individual operators that
are interacting with the process being controlled. The third level
describes the company managers responsible for the companies’
policy and strategies, and for the supervision of the operators’
activities. The second level describes activities of regulators and
associations responsible for monitoring the activities of the
companies in the aviation sector. The first level details activities of
the government and juridical aspects related to the same sector.
The representation of all these levels is necessary because they all
interact with each other, mutually directing and influencing each
level behavior, to control the system and provide adaptation to
environmental changes. To understand the events at any particular
level, it is therefore important to understand what has gone on at
all levels in the system. As pointed out by Rasmussen and Svedung
(2000), any sociotechnical system is subject to severe environ-
mental pressure in a dynamic society. The society pressure first
appears at the higher levels in the definition of legislation, regu-
lations, budget, and so forth, going down to the companies’ policies
and strategies, reaching the sharp-end operators’ behaviors and
actions. Adequate control strategies at all levels enable the system
to operate at low risk, in which a proper co-ordination and deci-
sion-making at all levels can be achieved. These observations are
particularly important considering the rapid growth of commercial
fixed wing flights in Brazil, which is increasing the demands on the
entire Brazilian air traffic system.
The regulation of Brazilian air traffic system complies with the
international ICAO legislation. The Brazilian constitution is
the source for the development of specific regulations governing
the functioning of the Brazilian Air Space Control System (SISCEAB),
which is also regulated by the Department of Air space Control
(DECEA), responsible for the integrated air defense and air traffic
control system. The SISCEAB encompasses all of the system’s
operational functions: communication, air traffic control, air
defense, aeronautic meteorology, cartography, aeronautic infor-
mation, search and rescue, and accident investigation.
Brazilian authorities decided to have only one ATM system, the
Integrated Air Defense and Air Traffic Control System (SISDACTA).
The SISDACTA is responsible for the co-ordination and joint opera-
tion of the air defense and air traffic control activities. The SISDACTA
acts on the entire Brazilian air space and in the international regions
under Brazilian responsibility, according to ICAO agreements. The
total area covered corresponds to 22 million square kilometers
(8 511 965 Km2 above Brazilian land). The SISDACTA comprises:
1) Tower Control Centers (TWRs) – control air traffic up to 5 km
from the airports;
2) Approach Control Centers (APPs) – control air traffic between 5
and 74 km from the airports;
3) Area Control Centers (ACCs) – known as CINDACTAs (acronym
for Integrated Air Defense and Air Traffic Control Centers)
control air traffic in the airways.
For radar surveillance and air traffic control purposes, Brazil has
been divided into 4 big regions. All radar data from each region are
processed by an area control center (ACC) or CINDACTA. Each one of
the four CINDACTAs has radar control of its region and is respon-
sible for the co-ordination of the regional air traffic. Due to its
centralized localization, CINDACTA I (Brasilia ACC), which operates
from Brasilia and was the first center installed, is responsible for
controlling most of the air traffic in Brazil. CINDACTA II operates in
the south region, CINDACTA III covers the northeast air space, and
CINDACTA IV (Amazon ACC) the north region, comprising the
Amazon forest. The CINDACTAs control traffic of en route aircraft,
when the airplane is above 19.500 feet. During the approach to
airports, air traffic is controlled by the Approach Control Centers
(APPs), and the final descent is controlled by the airport’s Tower
Control Center (TWRs).
P.V. Rodrigues de Carvalho et al. / Applied Ergonomics 40 (2009) 325–340
327

In each CINDACTA, there are two air traffic control systems in
two different control rooms:
– Military Operations Centers (COPM) that handle flight control
of military aircraft that are in military operation, operated by
military controllers, under military rules.
– Area Control Centers (ACCs), responsible for the air traffic
control of aircraft (civilian and military) flying under the
general aviation circulation rules, operated by civilian and
military controllers, under civilian rules.
Military controllers operate both centers. The integration of air
space and air defense control in Brazil – an option that is not used in
most countries – enables the use of the same resources for
communication, detection, surveillance, control and early warning
for air traffic control and for air space defense purposes.
4. The facts – what happened?
The collision was a very brief event. About 1 hour elapsed
from the moment when flight N600XL, the Embraer E-145 Legacy
jet, entered the air space controlled by the Brasilia ACC (CIN-
DACTA I), until the collision with flight GLO1907, the Boeing 737-
800, in the surveillance intersection zone between the Amazon
and Brasilia control centers. Fig. 2 shows the collision path. Flight
N600XL, even with some damage, was able to land at a military
base airport.
Fig. 3 shows a time-line with the main events before and after
the collision. Flight N600XL, an Embraer Legacy executive jet, took
off from Sa˜o Jose dos Campos airport (in Sa˜o Paulo state) at 14:30
(Brasilia time) to Eduardo Gomes airport in Manaus (in Amazonas
state) with a crew of two (pilot and co-pilot) and 4 passengers.
Flight N600XL’s plan stipulated two level changes (see Fig. 3).
However, the flight level stipulated for the first leg (until Brasilia) of
the flight, 370 (or 37 000 ft), was reached at 15:33 and was main-
tained up to the moment of the collision.
Flight GLO1907, the Boeing 737-800, took off from Eduardo
Gomes airport in Manaus to Brasilia International Airport at 15:35.
At 15:58, it reached the 370 flight level in the UZ6 airway (a dual
lane airway with 1000 ft of vertical separation between lanes), and
maintained this level, as stipulated by its flight plan, up to the
collision moment.
The last successful bilateral contact between the N600XL and
the Brasilia ACC happened at 15:51. At 15:55, the N600XL flew over
the Brasılia VOR vertical line, and entered the UZ6 airway (the same
as flight 1907, but in the opposite direction), without requesting or
receiving any instruction from the Brasilia traffic control center and
keeping the 370 flight level. At 16:02, the Brasilia ACC lost the
secondary surveillance radar (SSR) information about the N600XL,
which presents accurate altitude information to the traffic
controller. At 16:30, there was a 2 min loss of primary radar contact
with the N600XL, which transmits the aircraft’s geographic posi-
tion to the controller. No contact was attempted between 15:51 and
16:26 by either the N600XL or the Brasilia traffic control center.
From 16:26 to 16:53the Brasilia ACC made seven unsuccessful call
attempts. At 16:38, the Brasilia center lost definitively the primary
radar contact with the N600XL (it should have been transferred to
the Amazon control center).
The N600XL, at 16:48, began a series of 12 call attempt to the
Brasilia ACC. At 16:53:39, the N600XL was able to hear the last
(unilateral) call by the Brasilia center, instructing the N600XL to call
the Amazon ACC, but the crew was not able to copy the frequencies
provided. At 16:53:57, the N600XL radioed the Brasilia center
requesting the repetition the decimals of the first frequency,
because it had not been able to copy these values. The Brasilia
center did not receive this message. After that, the N600XL made
seven more unsuccessful call attempts to the Brasilia center from
16:54 to 16:57.
National
Congress
Min. of Defense
Air Force Command
Min. of
Planing
Min. of
Finance
Min. of
Labor
Air Space Control
Dept. - DECEA
Area Control Centers (ACCs)
–CINDACTA I, II III, IV
Civil Aviation National
Agency - ANAC
Aeronautic Infrastructure,
Airports Administration -
INFRAERO
Control System of Brazilian
Air Space - SISCEAB
International Civil Aviation
Organization – ICAO
Air Defense and Traffic Control
System – SISDACTA
Civil Controllers
Workers Association
TWRs Civil
Controllers
1. Government,
Legislation &
Budgeting
3. Companies
2. Associations
4. Operators
2. Regulatory
Bodies
CINDACTA Controllers
Workers Association
Tower Control
Centers (TWRs)
Approximation
Control Centers (APPs)
APPs Civil
Controllers
ACCs Civilian and Military
Controllers
Military Operations Control
Centers (COPM) –
CINDACTA I, II III, IV
COPMs Military
Controllers
Fig. 1. Generic actor map of Brazilian ATM system.
P.V. Rodrigues de Carvalho et al. / Applied Ergonomics 40 (2009) 325–340
328

The mid-air collision occurred when both aircraft were in the
UZ6 airway, flying in opposite directions, in the same lane – FL370
or 37000 ft – at 16:56:54. With the collision, flight 1907 became
uncontrollable, immediately going into a dive until it crashed into
the ground, causing the death of all passengers and crew members
(154 people), while flight N600XL was still able to fly and suc-
ceeded in making an emergency landing at the Brigadeiro Veloso
military air base.
5. The analysis – how and why it happened
The commander of the Department of Air space Control
(DECEA), responding to a senator’s question in the senate public
hearing after the accident said: ‘‘I am as puzzled as you Sir. A thing
like this is impossible to happen.’’
5.1. Preliminary investigation questions
The preliminary report elaborated by CENIPA (Ferreira, 2006)
indicated the following important things that did not happen in this
accident
There was no loss of radar surveillance between the Amazon
ACC (CINDACTA IV) and flight 1907, until its transference to the
Brasilia ACC;
There is no evidence in the communication records of any
N600XL request to the air traffic control centers to change its
flight level, after having reached the 370 flight level.
There is no registered evidence regarding any instruction for
the N600XL to change its flight level coming from air traffic
control, after the last successful bilateral contact (15:51)
between this aircraft and Brasilia center.
There is no registered evidence of any traffic alert alarm or
instruction for evasive action to the respective crews to avoid
collision in the TCAS systems, existing in both aircraft.
There is no registered evidence of any manifestation in either
crew related to any possible visual perception of the
approaching aircraft.
There is no attempt for action or evasive maneuver, according
to the data existing in flight recorders.
Looking through the list it seems that the entire system
(including
man-pilots and
controllers –
technology
and
TERES
NABOL
UZ6
UZ6
MANAUS
BRASÍLIA
W
E
N
S
Surveillance
intersection zone
between Amazon
and Brasilia control
centers
Legacy
Boeing
Fig. 2. The mid-air collision occurred approximately 20 Km northwest from Nabol, flight level 370 (37 000 ft), geographic coordinates 10 440 S/053 310 W, at 16:56:54 Brazilian
time. Both aircraft were flying in the UZ6 airway in opposite directions at the same altitude when the left wing of the Legacy, flight N600XL to Manaus, collided with left wing of the
Boeing 737, flight GLO1907 to Brasilia. (source Ferreira, 2006).
N600XL
1907
NABOL
TERES
BRASÍLIA
1555
SÃO JOSÉ
1451
Requested N600XL levels
MANAUS
1535
position &
time
Flight level
1533
360
370
380
1551
Last bilateral
contact
370
1657
Lost SSR
information
1602
From 1626 until collision many contact attempts
UZ6 airway
Lost of primary
radar contact
1638
1654
Unilateral contact:
change freq.
Fig. 3. Time-line with the main events before and after the collision.
P.V. Rodrigues de Carvalho et al. / Applied Ergonomics 40 (2009) 325–340
329

organization) was not aware that two aircraft were flying in the
same lane and in opposite directions.
5.2. Some official explanations
Charles Perrow pointed out that conventional explanations for
accidents are ‘‘operator error, faulty design or equipment, lack of
attention to safety features, lack of operating experience, inadequately
trained personnel, failure to use the most advanced technology, and
systems that are too big, under financed, or poorly run’’ (Perrow, 1984,
pp 63). The conclusions from the Senate Public Hearing report
confirm Perrow’s view and leans toward faulting the operators:
‘‘(.) it is possible to learn that several factors contributed to the
accident: possible technical failures, pilot failures and air traffic
controller failures. However, analyzing the event, I conclude that the
human factor is the main cause. Eliminating the human errors from
the causal chain, the accident would never have happened (.)’’
(Senado Federal, 2007, pp 61).
We want to go beyond these obvious explanations to consider
the complexity of the events, understanding the details of each
situation (how and why things happened). Our aim is to uncover
the underlying cognitive and organizational dynamics that enabled
the emergence of the tragedy, using the rich set of data that was
made available by the accident investigations as a window to grasp
a more fundamental understanding about the ATC system’s normal
functioning. To do so, in the following sections, we will discuss
HOW and WHY the safety barriers against mid-air collisions
described in Section 4 eroded enough to allow the emergence of
this accident.
5.3. The controlled air space
The controlled air space – the first safety barrier – is an abstract
construction, which aims to create ‘‘airways’’ in the space by
prescribing routes, altitudes, and directions to be followed by air
traffic. These airways are represented on aeronautical navigation
charts and should be followed by pilots and controllers. The flight
plan is the artifact that enables pilots and controllers to virtually
construct a flight, allocate a portion of the controlled air space to it,
and then discipline the flights and air traffic control.
In this accident, the flight levels prescribed in flight N600XL’s
flight plan were not followed. How could this happen? We will use
the investigation findings and human decision-making theories to
explain how normal pilots’ and controllers’ cognitive behaviors lead
to this unwanted system outcome.
5.3.1. Pilots’ behavior
Fig. 3 includes a representation of the prescribed altitudes of
flight N600XL’s approved flight plan. Starting in Sa˜o José dos
Campos, it passed through Poços de Caldas, in the UW2 airway at
flight level 370 (37000 ft) until Brasilia, where it would drop to
flight level 360 (36 000 ft) and enter the UZ6 airway. The flight plan
called for another altitude change at the Teres (virtual) notification
point, after which the aircraft would continue in the UZ6 airway,
but at FL380 (38 000 ft). The UZ6 is a dual lane airway with 1000 ft
vertical separation distances, in which the odd flight levels (370,
390, 410) are used for north-south navigation (Manaus to Brasılia),
and the even flight levels (360, 380, 420) are used for south-north
navigation (Brasılia to Manaus). According to the CPI final report
(Câmara dos Deputados, 2007), the submitted flight plan was
approved without modification by the Brasilia ACC.
Flight N600XL’s pilots had never flown in Brazil before
September 29, when they came to Sa˜o Paulo to fly the brand new
Embraer Legacy aircraft just acquired by American Excelaire. The
flight plan they were using was both prepared and submitted by
Embraer to the air traffic control center for approval. This flight plan
preparation procedure (a normal way to prepare flight plans), did not
require the pilots to look at Brazilian airways on the local aero-
nautical charts to configure their flight. As a consequence, some of
the details of the plan, and in particular, the situation regarding the
UZ6 airway, may not have come to the pilots’ attention.
The dialog in Table 1 shows the communications between flight
N600XL’s pilot and the TWR controller in Sa˜o Jose (SP) just before
departure.
The dialog above seems to be a normal departure communica-
tion between a tower controller and a pilot. In fact, we note that,
according to the basic verbal protocol rules, there are communi-
cation feedbacks and redundancies when flight N600XL’s pilot did
repeat the authorizations received. However, when the air traffic
controller did not communicate the complete flight plan (level
changes at Brasilia to 360 and at Teres to 380), (. ATC clearance to
Eduardo Gomes, flight level three seven zero .), the pilot did not
challenge the ATC’s clearance communication asking for the
clearance limits - until where he would fly at level 370 – implying
that he would fly at FL370 all the way to Eduardo Gomes airport, in
Manaus (against the flight plan information).
Table 1
Dialog between TWR controller and Legacy N600XL pilots – dialog in English.
Time
Operator
Communications
14:26:40 Legacy
Pilot
Sa˜o José ground november six zero zero x-ray lima.
14:26:47 TWR
controller
November six zero zero x-ray lima go ahead.
14:26:51 Legacy
pilot
Yes sir (.) start engines.
14:26:59 TWR
controller
Er, did you request, er, about weather?
14:27:02 Legacy
pilot
Yes sir, weather and runway.
14:27:05 TWR
controller
Roger. Er, Sa˜o José operating under visual conditions, ceiling
five thousand feet, visibility one zero kilometers, runway in
use one five, wind two two zero degrees, eight knots, quiu
eneiti one zero one nine, temperature two zero, time check
two five.
14:27:37 Legacy
pilot
Thank you.
14:31:46 Legacy
pilot
Ground, november six zero zero x-ray lima like to have push
back for taxi.
14:32:02 Legacy
pilot
Ground, november six zero zero lima, x-ray lima, like to give
ready, clear to push for taxi.
14:32:10 TWR
controller
Ah, november six zero zero x-ray lima, er, clear to start up,
temperature two zero. Er, are you ready to taxi?
14:32:24 Legacy
pilot
Yes sir, we’ll be in turn right now (.) to the taxi back.
14:32:31 TWR
controller
Er, report ready for taxi.
14:32:34 Legacy
pilot
Report ready to taxi, six hundred x-ray lima.
14:40:31 Legacy
pilot
Sa˜o José ground, november six zero zero x-ray lima ready to
taxi.
14:40:38 TWR
controller
Er, roger. Er, maintain position, november six zero zero x-ray
lima.
14:40:44 Legacy
pilot
November six zero zero x-ray lima maintaining position.
14:41:50 TWR
controller
Are you ready to copy the clearance?
14:41:53 Legacy
Ah, affirmative, yes.
14:41:57 TWR
controller
November six zero zero x-ray lima, ATC clearance to Eduardo
Gomes, flight level three seven zero, direct Poços de Caldas,
squawk transponder code four five seven four. After take-off
perform OREN departure.
14:42:26 Legacy
Pilot
Okay sir, I get the runway one five to so. ah SBEG, flight level
three seven zero, I didn’t get the first fix, I get squawk four
five seven four, OREN departure.
Source: CPI final report (Câmara dos Deputados, 2007).
P.V. Rodrigues de Carvalho et al. / Applied Ergonomics 40 (2009) 325–340
330

From this dialog, we conclude that the N600XL pilots had two
conflicting sets of information at the moment of departure: 1) the
flight plan with many level changes, and 2) the ATC’s communi-
cation clearance that mentioned only FL370. Based on the con-
flicting information (inputs) they had, why did the pilots not
evaluate and compare the inputs and query the controller?
Traditional research in decision-making has developed norma-
tive models for human decisions that involve: first, generate a range
of options, then generate a set of criteria for evaluating these
options, assign weights for the evaluation criteria, rate each option
on each evaluation criterion, perform calculations, and finally,
compare the options determining the best choice (Doherty, 1993). If
people during their normal work made decisions according to this
model, we could imagine that the pilots’ actual behavior was
completely unusual or abnormal (and probably very unlikely to
happen again with other pilots), because their decision process did
not agree with any of the normative steps described.
However, situated or naturalistic based human decision-making
models, identifying deviations from the norms, e.g. Prospect Theory
(Tversky and Kahneman, 1974) suggest that decision-makers
generally apply a wide range of heuristics, even when these result
in sub-optimal performance. Heuristic-based theories have been
superseded by descriptive models emphasizing the phenomena
themselves, without reference to abstract norms, such as Ras-
mussen’s model of Cognitive Control (Rasmussen, 1983) and Klein’s
Recognition-Primed Decision-Making - RPD model (Klein, 1993)
and more recently the efficiency-thoroughness trade-off, the ETTO
principle (Hollnagel, 2004). In such models, rather than making
a concurrent evaluation of the relative advantages and disadvan-
tages of several courses of action (the normative approach), the
decision-makers in actual situations select a course of action, which
is generated through heuristics and recognition. A situation similar
to a previous experience, a familiar situation, is evaluated for its
adequacy in the particular set of circumstances of the present
situation. There are many empirical findings that even in safety-
critical systems, for instance, nuclear systems (Carvalho, 2005;
Carvalho et al., 2005; Carvalho et al., 2006), experienced operators
use pattern recognition and simple heuristics to make decisions
during their daily work in operating the system.
Flight N600XL’s pilots’ behavior – not querying the ATC
controller, not searching for more information – indicates that they
decided based on the recognition that the clearance they received
from ATC is a familiar situation of changing a flight plan by ATC
authorities. Together with their non-familiarity with Brazilian air
space, the pilots did not have any doubt about the new flight plan
they got after the ATC clearance communication. The pilots
confirmed that they had no doubts in an interview for the Brazilian
newspaper Folha de Sa˜o Paulo on Feb. 19, 2007. The pilot said, ‘‘It is
common for there to be differences, it happens all the time. You have
to fly according to the authorization. The actual flight plan is the
clearance that you receive from the control center.’’, and the co-pilot,
‘‘As he said, it happens all the time, we have a flight plan to fly at one
altitude and we are authorized to fly at another one. Let me say that
this happens 99% of the time. The flight plan is just a proposal.’’
This behavior can also be explained according to the belief-bias
effect that occurs in reasoning when people make judgments based
on prior beliefs and general knowledge, rather than on the rules of
logic and the information available (Evans, 2004; Quinn and Mar-
kovits, 1998). In general, people are likely to make inadequate
choices when the logic of a reasoning problem (conflicting infor-
mation about the flight plan) is not supported by their background
knowledge (flight plan changes happen all the time) (Holyoak and
Simon, 1999).
Then, an unwanted system outcome – an aircraft flying at
a wrong level in a dual lane airway – was made possible by normal
behaviors of the pilots. Normal in Perrow’s sense, not desired or
expected, but a perfectly possible outcome of the system.
5.3.2. Air traffic controller behavior
From other side, the air traffic controller gave a flight authori-
zation where only the level of the first leg (FL370) was communi-
cated in a route that included two other level changes. The ICA
100-12 publication (DECEA, 2006) regulates the air traffic services
in Brazil. Chapter 8, Area Control Services, defines the rules to be
applied for ATC services. According to these rules, the submitted
flight plan can be different from the actual plan, and the controller
must indicate the various levels of the route, or the limit for the
route authorization he gave. In the ICA 100-12 there are the
following definitions:
Flight plan – specific Information, related to a planned flight or
part of a flight of an aircraft, supplied to the air traffic services.
Submitted flight plan – flight plan such as it is submitted by the
pilot, or his representative, to the air traffic services.
Current flight plan – actual flight plan, including the modifi-
cations (if they were necessary) made by air traffic services.
Based on theses rules and controller behavior the Congressional
Hearing Report concludes ‘‘(.) a message with partial authorization
for the flight of an aircraft is a procedure without any normative
support’’ (Câmara dos Deputados, 2007, p. 58).
The procedure used for flight plan authorization has many steps.
First, the plan is submitted in the Air Information Service Room,
located in the departure airport. In this room, a Sergeant specialized
in aeronautic information (not a flight controller) receives and
checks the submitted plan. Next, the plan is presented to another
Sergeant, specialized in communications, who inputs the flight
plan data into the computerized system and sends these data to the
regional ACC, where a flight controller in the Flight Plan Room
checks the proposed route comparing it to the other flight plans in
the region. If it is OK, he/she confirms the insertion of the flight plan
data in the system and sends the ‘‘Traffic Authorization or Clearance
Delivery’’ by an electronic message to the departure airport. Finally,
the TWR controller at the departure airport, responsible for the
‘‘traffic authorization’’, using a private telephone line (called ‘‘hot
line’’), calls the ACC controller to confirm the flight plan informa-
tion for the clearance delivery. Having received the ACC’s confir-
mation, the TWR controller radios the ‘‘Authorized Flight Plan’’ to
the flight’s pilot.
The last internal step of this procedure, the final communication
between flight controllers (Sa˜o Jose TWR controller with the Bra-
silia ACC) to confirm flight N600XL’s information is transcribed in
Table 2.
In this dialog, which happened about 10 min before the take-off
clearance communication dialog between the TWR controller and
the pilot, presented in Table 1, the controllers’ verbalizations refer
only to the level 370. In both dialogs, there are no explicit verbal-
izations regarding the complete N600XL flight plan. In all conver-
sations involving pilots, the TWR controller in Sa˜o Jose, and the ACC
controller in Brasilia, the level changes in N600XL’s flight plan were
not verbalized. Therefore, from this dialog, and the dialog between
TWR controller and the pilot, the controllers did not follow the
DECEA prescriptions.
The easiest way to address this situation is consider that we
have a human error. Doing so, the problem can be confined to those
specific controllers that behave in a completely different way than
the other controllers working in the system.
However, from a systemic point of view, this can also be viewed
as a normal behavior. On long routes, with many level changes, the
ATC controller may assume that pilots are aware of the need for
P.V. Rodrigues de Carvalho et al. / Applied Ergonomics 40 (2009) 325–340
331

level changes during the flight. They also may think that there will
be other communication opportunities later on, when the aircraft
reaches the clearance limits (in this flight the first one was over
Brasilia), at which time the pilots will receive the information
regarding the other legs of the plan.
Both behaviors – pilots’ and controllers’ – can be explained by
the by the efficiency-thoroughness trade-off, the ETTO principle
(Hollnagel, 2004). Pilots and controllers use common and simple
heuristics such as ‘‘someone one will communicate this later’’, ‘‘there
is no need to pay attention to that now’’, ‘‘flight plans can always be
changed’’ that characterize the ETTO principle. Initially conceived to
explain coping strategies to reduce the cognitive task demands (e.g.
time pressure, information overload etc.), the ETTO principle is
currently viewed as a typical strategy to cope with complexity in
a long-term perspective (Hollnagel and Woods, 2005). Using ETTO
heuristics it is possible to reduce cognitive effort and keep spare
capacity for emergency situations, making a trade-off before it is
objectively required by the current situation. Thus, the choice of
a coping strategy not only represents a short term or temporary
adjustment but may equally well indicate a more permanent way to
do things in the system functioning. The risks inherent in these
strategies use are obvious, as can be seen from their part in this
accident explanation.
We argue that the controlled air space as a cognitive system
(a system that involves people, technology and organization) may be
a weaker safety barrier than we usually expect, enabling unwanted
system outcomes that may result in system accidents, depending on
how the system is functioning daily. Due to the limitations inherent in
analyzing only one case, we are not able do know how frequently the
ETTO based strategies are actually being used in the entire Brazilian
ATC system. However, if they are frequent we may have a serious
safety problem, since the first barrier against mid-air collisions may
be not working properly, characterizing a sociotechnical drift-
to-failure situation (Snook, 2000; Rasmussen and Svedung, 2000). In
this particular case, pilots and air traffic controllers, all acting nor-
mally (albeit in this case inadequately in hindsight), interacted with
each other in a way that to all appearances – in foresight – was
normal, but allowed a mid-air collision to occur.
5.4. The TCAS functioning
Another barrier used to avoid mid-air collisions is the Traffic
Alert and Collision Avoidance System (TCAS). Both aircraft involved
in this accident were equipped with TCAS equipment. According to
Brazilian flight regulations, before entering air space with reduced
vertical separation minimum (RVSM space, where the vertical
separation distance is 1000 ft.), the pilot must check (besides other
equipment) that the transponder is normally working in mode C or
S. In Mode C, the transponder informs its code and the aircraft’s
altitude with good accuracy. In Mode S, in addition to the Mode C
information, the transponder sends other flight parameters such as
speed, direction etc. The TCAS requires the transponder function to
operate, and goes into standby mode (effectively off) whenever the
transponder is not enabled.
One of the most puzzling notes of the accident preliminary
investigation report is that ‘‘There was no attempt for action or
evasive maneuver, according to the data existing in flight recorders’’
(Ferreira, 2006), indicating that TCAS did not give the TA (Traffic
Advisory) or RA (Resolution Advisory) alarms to the pilots.
According to the time-line presented in Fig. 3, Brasilia ACC failed to
receive any transponder replies from flight N600XL for approxi-
mately the last 50 min of the flight before the collision.
Further investigations indicated that the transponder and TCAS
of flight GLO1907 was working properly during the accident, and
the transponder of flight N600XL was not functioning (it was in the
OFF mode) at the moment of the collision. Evidence that flight
N600XL’s transponder was in the OFF mode came from many
sources:
– The lack of N600XL transponder contact with the Brasilia ACC;
– The lack of N600XL transponder contact with the Amazon ACC;
– The dialog between N600XL pilots registered in the CVR
(Cockpit Voice Recorder), just after the collision.
This dialog is presented in Table 3 below.
Clearly, the Transponder/TCAS system of the N600XL aircraft
failed. However, even after extensive tests, teardowns and simu-
lations in the aircraft manufacturer and in the transponder/TCAS
manufacturer, the reason why the transponder was set in the
STAND BY mode (turned off) remains ‘‘unexplained’’ to this day.
Three main possibilities were investigated: 1) hardware/technical
failure, 2) pilots deliberately turned off the transponder, and 3)
pilots accidentally turned it off with an unintentional slip. About
the hardware/technical failure, all post-accident information made
public up to this moment – the investigation has not finished yet –
indicates that the critical transponder components were opera-
tional. The second possibility, a professional crew willingly
switching off such an important piece of equipment in the RVSM
space would have been an act of basic procedure violation (Reason,
1997). This behavior simply could not be admitted by any profes-
sional crew and has no support in the flight data collected (Câmara
dos Deputados, 2007). The last possibility, pilots accidentally
turning off the TCAS with an unintentional slip, is also not sup-
ported by the investigation findings. The only known way to put the
transponder STAND BY mode is to press the button on the Radio
Management Unit (RMU) control screen twice, within less than
20 s, as shown in Fig. 4, which could not be attributed to a slip.
In his testimony to the Senate Congressional Hearing, the official
responsible for the Brazilian aeronautic accident investigation said,
‘‘In accordance with the description documents and its certifications,
the transponder and TCAS systems did not present design or integra-
tion errors. They functioned as they had to function. (.)We now focus
our investigation on the operational factor, or either, in the relation of
the operation of. of the human being with that system’’.
In another part of this testimony, he added some new infor-
mation related to the ‘‘operational factor’’: ‘‘The transponder stopped
functioning. We did all the tests to try to exclude the possibility of
a technical failure in the transponder. We did not find technical
Table 2
Dialog between TWR and ACC controllers – Dialog in Portuguese, translated to
English.
Time
Operator
Communications
14:33:33
Brasilia ACC
Controller
Talk Sa˜o Jose
14:33:35
Sa˜o Jose TWR
controller
Hi Brasilia. The November six zero zero x-ray lima to
Eduardo Gomes Sa˜o Jose Eduardo Gomes .
requesting level three seven zero.
14:33:50
Brasilia ACC
Controller
. Level three seven zero transponder four five seven
four Poços de Caldas.
14:33:55
Sa˜o Jose TWR
controller
Three seven zero direction Poços. What is the
frequency he calls you there?
14:33:59
Brasilia ACC
Controller
One two six fifteen . one three three five
14:34:04
Sa˜o Jose TWR
controller
One three three five three seven zero direction Poços.
OK
14:34:09
Brasilia ACC
Controller
Bye bye
14: 34:10
Sa˜o Jose TWR
controller
Bye bye
Source: CPI final report (Câmara dos Deputados, 2007).
P.V. Rodrigues de Carvalho et al. / Applied Ergonomics 40 (2009) 325–340
332

malfunctions. The equipment functioned according to its specifica-
tions. Therefore, we are focusing on the operational factor. What I have
to say at this moment is that I have no indication that the pilots turned
off the transponder intentionally. I do not have, looking at the CVR, I
have nothing that indicates an action related to this equipment.’’
(Câmara dos Deputados, 2007).
However, the transponder was in OFF mode and TCAS was in
STAND BY mode for 50 min before the collision. The question that
remains is: Why did the pilots not perceive that these important
pieces of equipment were not functioning properly? The indica-
tions in the cockpit that the transponder is currently switched off,
and as a consequence the TCAS is in STAND BY mode, are difficult to
perceive as shown in Fig. 5, because they are not indicated in the
standard failure colors, and there is no alarm signal to draw the
pilots’ attention. The indication that the TCAS is in standby mode
appears on the Radio Management Unit (RMU) as a small indication
in green letters inside a yellow window, just underneath the
transponder code selected. There are two other indications
regarding the operation mode of the TCAS: on the right side of the
Primary Function Display (PFD) there is a message in white and
small letters indicating TCAS OFF, and on the left side of the Multi
Function Display (MFD) (if this specific page is the selected one) the
message is repeated in the same small white letters (see Fig. 5).
Problems with these indications had already been noted, back in
2005, by the European air traffic authorities, in a similar avionic
system: ‘‘When this reversion to standby mode occurs, the ATC/TCAS
standby mode is indicated on the RMU and Cockpit Displays (PFD/
MFD), however these indications may not be apparent to pilots,
especially during periods of high workload’’ (Irish Aviation Authority,
2005, pp 2). Note that in spite of the observation dated from 2005
that the indications may not be apparent to pilots, the same indi-
cation methods are still used in avionic systems.
There were other cases of spurious failures or automation
surprises in Transponder/TCAS operation in a similar avionic
system. At the end of 2003, the European ATC providers noted
many lost transponder tracks for several minutes. An Embraer
E-145 remained invisible in the busy European air space for more
than 45 min before it was identified as an ‘‘unknown target’’ by
French military control, and the first steps for intercepting the
target were initiated. The inquiry identified a software problem in
a particular transponder type and the European Aircraft Safety
Agency (EASA) issued an Airworthiness Directive (AD) in August
2005. According to this AD, ‘‘A design deficiency causes the tran-
sponder to revert to standby mode if a change of the 4096 ATC code
(also called the Mode A code) is not completed within 5 seconds. As
a consequence, the SSR radar symbol and label associated with the
aircraft’s position will no longer be shown on the ATC ground radar
display. In addition, aircraft collision avoidance systems (ACAS) on
board own and other aircraft will be compromised. Current opera-
tional procedures, typically, do not require the crew to recheck the
transponder status after changing the 4096 ATC Code. This type of
failure will increase ATC workload and will result in improper func-
tioning of ACAS’’ (EASA, 2005, p. 2).
Although the specific transponder failure mentioned above has
already been corrected in the new transponders’ software versions,
we can conclude this section observing that, even without cata-
strophic technical or operational failures having been identified in
flight N600XL’s transponder after more than a year of investiga-
tions in different organizations involving two countries, on
September-29-2006 an aircraft remained invisible for about 50 min
in the Brasilia RVSM controlled air space, without a transponder
signal. Therefore, we add normal equipment to the Dekker (2006)
citation. Therefore, in complex systems, accidents occur with
‘‘Normal people working in normal organizations, with normal
equipment’’ considering that the transponder functioned (and it is
still functioning) in a normal way.
To conclude this point, we argue that a search-for-failure in the
transponder (mal) functioning or about human error in its
Table 3
Dialog in the Legacy cockpit at the moment of the collision – Dialog in English.
Time
Operator
Communication
16:56:38 Co-pilot on Legacy
radio
Brasılia, November six zero zero X-ray Lima.
16:56:50 Co-pilot on Legacy
radio
Brasılia, November six zero zero X-ray Lima.
16:56:54 Cockpit
microphone
Impact sound
16:56:56 Pilot, Co-pilot
Uh oh
16:56:56 Cockpit
microphone
Automatic pilot (.)
16:59:08 Co-pilot
This.
16:59:12 Pilot or Co-pilot
Deep breath sound
16:59:13 Co-pilot
Man, are you with TCAS on?
16:59:15 Pilot
The TCAS is off
16:59:25 Co-pilot
All right, pay attention only at the traffic. We will
succeed, we will succeed we will succeed. I know
that.
Just after the collision, the co-pilot started a visual flight (. pay attention only at the
traffic .) which make sense only in the absence of the TCAS/transponder. After
that, when the pilot set the emergency code 7700 the transponder worked well.
17:02:06 Pilot
I will set to seven thousand and seven hundred. It is
an emergency!
17:02:08 Co-pilot
Yes, set it up.
In the following dialog presents some comments of the crew just after the collision,
which shows some issues about the crew’s rationale.
17:28:36 Pilot
So. if we beat in somebody. I want to say, we were in
the right altitude.
17:28:39 Co-pilot
Well, they were trying to give the frequency and I was
trying to answer them. I only got three numbers. I did
not get the last two, then I was calling all the
frequencies.
17:28:47 Co-pilot
They were probably trying to induce us to go down.
But, probably, we were not in the radar. Or they .
(fucks), and they did not make.
17:28:54 Passenger
(Embraer
employee)
At any moment we receive a clearance to leave this
altitude. Then I left in the altitude.
17:28:59 Co-pilot
The guys forgot us. Previous frequency forgot us
completely. And I started to risk. This is not right. I
was without speaking with somebody for too much
time.
17:29:11 Co-pilot
We are alive.
17:29:13 Pilot
But, I am worried about the other airplane. If we beat
in another airplane (.) whatever more this can be?
Source Câmara dos Deputados, 2007.
Fig. 4. Legacy cockpit and RMU units with transponder desativation procedure and the
indication of the TCAS in STAND BY on the RMU. (source Câmara dos Deputados, 2007).
P.V. Rodrigues de Carvalho et al. / Applied Ergonomics 40 (2009) 325–340
333

operation (as the traditional accident analysis does) would not be
sufficient to explain why such an important safe-critical function
(collision avoidance) remained out of operation without being
noticed during this collision. As already pointed out in Section 5.3,
the overall system function and the interactions agents perform in
a daily basis must be addressed to explain the mechanisms that
allow the function to fail.
5.5. The Brasilia ACC and their communications with flight N600XL
After the flight plan misunderstanding and the TCAS in standby
mode, flight N600XL was flying in the UZ6 dual lane airway in the
wrong direction near Brasilia. In this situation, the communication
loop between flight N600XL’s crew and the Brasilia ACC became the
last barrier to tragedy. The fundamental issues about the quality of
the communication feedback loops, and how they can affect system
safety have already been addressed (see Carvalho et al., 2007). In
the following sections, we will describe how and why this last
safety barrier did not function to avoid this accident.
5.5.1. What the Brasilia ACC controller actually saw
During the investigations, the representations of flight N600XL
on the Brasilia ACC controller radar screen were reproduced. We
use this information to show what the controller was actually
seeing as the flight progressed. In Fig. 6, we show a radar screenshot
of the Brasilia ACC. Flight N600XL, with its main flight data, is
represented by the target symbol and a data block.
The meaning of the data block is explained in Fig. 7.
The aircraft altitude information is located on the second line of
the data block. This line is composed of three segments: a three
digit number, a symbol, and another three digit number. The three
digits on the left side of the second line represent the aircraft’s
altitude (usually the last surveillance radar information). The
symbol between the numbers is a status indicator and specifies the
type of altitude information displayed by the digits to its left
(actual, estimated) and its immediate future development (stable,
climbing, descending). A ‘‘¼’’ (equals) symbol indicates level flight,
a ‘‘þ’’ (plus) symbol indicates a climbing aircraft, and a ‘‘À’’ (minus)
symbol indicates a descending aircraft. The display of the ¼, þ, or À
symbols also provides visual confirmation to the controller that the
aircraft’s transponder is providing Mode C or S altitude information
to air traffic control. Modes C and S of the transponder report
altitude data to air traffic control that is, in turn, displayed on the
radarscope in hundred foot increments. The Z (capital letter Z)
symbol indicates that the aircraft altitude is not transponder
reported but is estimated by the primary radar. The three digits on
the right hand side of the second line of the data block represent
the altitude authorized by the flight plan for that particular flight
segment.
Each controller is responsible for the surveillance of specific
sectors of the controlled air space that are displayed in his/her
workplace. The sectors are displayed on the controller radar screen
or radarscope. The screen of workplace 8 that controls sectors 7
(the sector of the collision), 8 and 9 at 15:55 (four minutes after
flight N600XL entered sector 7) is presented in Fig. 8. The colors
(the black background and white lines) were changed for better
Fig. 5. Indications of TCAS OFF in the PFD (left) and MFD (right). (source Câmara dos Deputados, 2007).
+
X
NX600XL
197+370
35 S017W
ROCHO
TEXAS
BCO
Legacy target
position symbol
Fig. 6. A schematic view of the Brasilia ACC radar screen.
P.V. Rodrigues de Carvalho et al. / Applied Ergonomics 40 (2009) 325–340
334

visualization on paper. On the radar screen, in addition to the
targets information (aircraft flying) the controller also has the flight
plan information in the electronic flight strips, displayed in the
vertical column on the right side of the screen.
The first changes in flight N600XL’s data block occurred when
the aircraft entered sector 7. It appeared to the controller as pre-
sented in Fig. 9 (with inverted colors).
At 16:01 flight N600XL’s transponder failed and the ACC screen
changed automatically to that presented in Fig. 10.
Even without the transponder information, the N600XL data
block continues on the screen because the system ‘‘understands’’
that the target detected by the primary radar is the same aircraft
that earlier transmitted the identification code. Therefore, the
N600XL data block continues on the screen based on the correla-
tion of the target position and the previous information received
from the secondary radar.
Under the Brazilian ATC regulations (ICA 100-12), when an air-
craft’s transponder ceases to present the required response signal
in the RVSM air space (the case of Brasilia Vertical line), the
controller must ask the pilot to verify the functioning of the tran-
sponder. Besides that, in the Reduced Vertical Separation Minimum
air space, a vertical separation of 1000ft is permitted only to
appropriately equipped aircraft (i.e., those with, among other
things, a functioning transponder that has Mode C and Mode S
capabilities). With the loss of the transponder signal, ATC was
required to suspend RVSM operations for flight N600XL and
provide at least 2000 ft of vertical separation (Non-RVSM separa-
tion in the Upper Control Area is 2000 ft) between it and other
traffic along its route of flight. This included flight 1907. Even
considering the importance that the loss of the transponder signal
has in the regulations and in the controller procedures, there is no
active warning signal delivered by the system. The controller must
actively seek for the information on the screen – the symbol
changes in the aircraft data blocks – as described above.
On the three screens presented in Fig. 11, we see the primary
radar indications with level variations, and when flight N600XL’s
target disappeared completely from the radar screen, before going
out of sector 7 (still without a transponder signal). The flight data
recorder confirms that flight N600XL remained level at Flight Level
370 until the collision. However, the information received from the
height-finding radar estimated the aircraft at a different altitude
with nearly every ten second sweep of the radarscope (at 16:29:58
the indicated level was 348). The estimated altitudes varied
considerably not only from the aircraft’s actual altitude, but also
from the flight’s planned altitude, depicted in the right-hand
portion of the second line of the data block. When it was reaching
TERES notification point, there was an intermittent detection from
primary radar, and flight N600XL disappeared from the controller
screen at 16:30:08 and appeared again at 16:31:28, as shown in
Fig. 11.
5.5.2. The communication problems
Table 4 summarizes the major events and communication
attempts that occurred in the Brasilia ACC regarding flight N600XL.
The communication frequencies used in the controlled air space
are divided according to the sector the aircraft is currently flying in
and are described in the aeronautical charts. Each aircraft must set
its equipment to a communications frequency from which it can
receive and transmit voice communications. From the other side,
the ACC control center transmits in broadcast, in all frequencies
activated for each sector. Therefore, its transmissions should be
received by all aircraft flying in the region. Table 5 shows the
frequencies used in sectors 7, 8 and 9, controlled by workstation 8
of the Brasilia ACC.
Based on the data presented in Tables 4 and 5 we note that the
communications difficulties were related to the frequencies
selected, not to flaws in the radio system. The first 6 attempts made
by Brasilia ACC used the frequency of 125.05 and were not received
in flight N600XL’s radio system. This occurred probably because
flight N600XL was reaching the limit of the communication range
of the sector 7 frequency, in need of a new frequency. Indeed, in his
second attempt, the controller tried to communicate a new
frequency to flight N600XL’s crew. Flight N600XL’s first attempts
used the 123.30 and 133.05 frequencies, which were not activated
in workplace 8. Finally, the last and only successful communication
occurred on the 135.90 frequency. However, at the moment of this
communication flight N600XL was leaving sector 7 and entering
the Amazon ACC region. The Brasilia ACC registered clearly the
Altitude status indicator
+ Aircraft climbing
– Aircraft descending
Aircraft level using transponder signals
= Level Flight
Modes C and S
Z Primary radar estimating aircraft altitude
? Mode C altitude momentarily lost
NX600XL
-------------- Call sign
Current altitude (MODE C) --------- 197 + 370
-------------- Flight planned altitude
Ground speed (x10) -------- 35 S017W ---------------- ACC sector ID
+ Target position symbol
+ Target detected by primary and secondary radar with velocity vector (MODE S transponder)
+ Target detected by primary and secondary radar (MODE C transponder)
+ Target detected only by primary radar, no transponder returns – primary radar only
Notification points
Fig. 7. The meaning of the data block in radarscope.
P.V. Rodrigues de Carvalho et al. / Applied Ergonomics 40 (2009) 325–340
335

communications of flight GLO1907 with the Amazon ACC just
before the collision, during ATC service transfer from the Amazon
ACC to Brasilia ACC, on the 125.20 frequency, which indicated that
the ATC radio system was working properly at that moment, in
a normal way.
5.5.3. The controllers’ inaction
As already noted long ago by Dailey (1984), ‘‘The central skill of
the controller seems to the ability to respond to a variety of
quantitative inputs about several aircraft simultaneously and to
form a continuously changing mental picture to be used as basis for
Fig. 8. A schematic view of the workplace 8 controller screen at 15:55 (source: Câmara dos Deputados, 2007).
N X600X L
370=370
45 S077W
TAM3823
052+370
16 S097W
S077W
+
+
X
15:55:23
NX600XL
370=360
45 S077W
TAM3823
053?370
16 S097W
S077W
+
X
15:55:28
+
+
+
Fig. 9. Part of the sector 7 screen at 15:55. Two minutes before the Legacy reached the Brasilia VOR (VHF Omnidirectional Range), the system automatically changed the Legacy’s
data block from 370 ¼ 370 to 370 ¼ 360 indicating the new flight level after Brasilia VOR contained in the filed flight plan. The ¼ sign indicates secondary surveillance radar
(transponder functioning) (source Câmara dos Deputados, 2007).
P.V. Rodrigues de Carvalho et al. / Applied Ergonomics 40 (2009) 325–340
336

planning and controlling the courses of the aircraft’’ (pp 134).
Therefore, the aim of the air traffic controller is to construct
a mental model that matches the dynamics and evolution of the air
traffic situation. This mental model is constructed by a continuous
comparison of the current air traffic situation with the anticipation
of the situation in the near future, to plan required actions. After
Endsley (1995), this special type of mental representation is called
Situation Awareness (SA). To achieve an adequate SA, the controller
must understand the current air traffic situation monitoring the
radarscope and other information sources (flight plans, air-charts,
communication frequencies), and anticipate the future aircraft
trajectories using sector maps, aircraft velocities and altitudes,
flight strips, communication with pilots and co-ordination with
other controllers. Therefore, the controller’s work activity requires
active monitoring and control strategies to cope with workload
variations, maintaining the SA that is paramount to ensure that the
mental picture remains consistent with the actual situation.
The dominant paradigm of the post hoc analysis of human error
in accidents indicates that many operational errors can be attrib-
uted to lack of SA or to SA problems (e.g. Jones and Endsley, 1996),
and in this accident the investigations reached similar conclusions.
According to the final report of the Senate investigation, the acci-
dent was caused by a chain of human errors (Senado Federal, 2007),
including the ATC controllers’ failures in providing adequate air
traffic services to flight N600XL to ensure that it was properly
separated from other traffic (flight GLO1907 included). These fail-
ures were related to monitoring the radarscope over an extended
period without perceiving the potential conflict situation, proce-
dure non-compliance (not terminating RVSM operation for flight
N600XL after the loss of Mode C transponder flight level informa-
tion), failures in the co-ordination with other controllers (e.g.
transfer of control between sectors should include any abnormal
communications status or uncertainties about flight data; incom-
plete relief briefings during shift changeovers), failures in taking
immediate actions (e.g. to locate an aircraft which has been
simultaneously or unexpectedly lost from radar and radio).
In fact, the main indications of ‘‘abnormal’’ behavior of flight
N600XL can be summarized as:
– Actual flight level different from the planned flight level;
– Transponder ceased to reply the ATC surveillance radar
(2 signals – Z letter in the data block and target symbol change);
– Erratic level variation (FL331to FL396);
– The disappearance of the aircraft inside sector 7 (before leaving
sector 7).
We note that after the first automatic change in the radarscope
(15:55), when flight N600XL entered in sector 7, the radarscope
indicated the loss of transponder returns (16:02), and differences
between the actual and planned flight levels. The controller on
duty, despite these screen changes, remained almost 22 minutes
without taking any action. After the shift changeover at worksta-
tion 8 (16:17), the substitute controller took 10 min to try, without
success, to contact flight N600XL’s crew.
16:01:43
GLO1693
062?400
22 S097W
+
+
X
GOLA
LUKA
ACRE
NATA
LATO
PORMT
REGEL
BILA
EROG
EDNAP
GLO1693
062?400
23 S097W
+
+
X
GOLA
LUKA
ACRE
NATA
LATO
PORMT
REGEL
BILA
EROG
EDNAP
NX600XL
370Z360
46 S077W
16:01:53
NX600XL
370=360
46 S077W
Fig. 10. Brasilia ACC system stop to receive replies from radar surveillance interrogations of the Legacy’s transponder. In the controller scope at 16:01:53 the circle disappeared from
the Legacy’s target position symbol, leaving only the þ to represent the aircraft. In place of the ¼ that previously showed the Legacy level at a reported Mode C altitude of Flight
Level 370, the altitude status indicator began to display a Z, indicating that the three numerals to the left of the Z represent the aircraft’s estimated altitude derived from information
supplied by ground-based height-finding primary radar (source Câmara dos Deputados, 2007).
16:29:58
EGOLA
TERES
DRD1451
380=380
47 S077W
+
NX600XL
348Z360
45 S077W
+
DRD1451
380=380
47 S077W
16:30:08
TERES
DRD1451
380=380
45 S077W
+
+
EGOLA
+
TERES
16:31:28
Fig. 11. N600XL with a big level variation (348) due to the low accuracy of the primary height-finding radar. The DRD1451 flight indicated that the ATC system was able to receive
transponder signals at that moment. At 16:30:08 the Legacy, still in the sector 7, disappeared from the radarscope, indicating that it is flying without the primary and secondary
radar systems. At 16:31:26 the N600XL appeared again in the screen. The data block is missing because it appeared as a non-correlated target (source Câmara dos Deputados, 2007).
P.V. Rodrigues de Carvalho et al. / Applied Ergonomics 40 (2009) 325–340
337

However, how much time is needed to perceive screen changes
and more importantly, consider that they are important enough to
act? Can other controllers working in the same system behave in
the same way? In such a cluttered display (Fig. 8 is only a small part
of the radar screen where target (aircraft) data blocks are super-
imposed on each other, making their visualization difficult), an
automatic change made by the system does not favor immediate
perception by the controller. In fact, the automatic change of the
‘‘cleared altitude’’ in the data block, without any controller action or
effective warning signal, can be easily overlooked by controllers
and is potentially misleading, especially if we consider that the
controller normally has his/her attention divided among several
data blocks (aircraft under his/her responsibility) on the screen.
Indeed, Means et al.’s (1988) cognitive task analysis indicates that
controllers are more aware of flight data of aircraft that they had
performed control actions on than of aircraft that they had not
carried out control actions on. Barber (1988), analyzing the mid-air
collision in Yugoslavia back in 1976, had already noted that in
a divided-attention task when people try to pay attention to two or
more simultaneous tasks, the consequences of divided-attention
could be disastrous. Perception of the environment characteristics
depends on the attention that allows our cognitive processes to
take in selected aspects of our sensory world in an efficient and
accurate manner (Palmer, 1999). Therefore, small screen changes
without warning, to be quickly perceived, require a concentration
of mental activity – attention – only on these inputs, which is very
difficult in divided-attention tasks.
The other issue is: Is perceiving the changes enough to trigger
a controller action? This depends on the strategies controllers
normally use to control their workload and cognitive demands.
Using controller interviews to study conflict resolution, Kallus et al.
(1999) found that under low workload, the controllers take more
time to solve conflicts than in high workload situations, because
under low workload they have enough time and prefer to monitor
aircraft movement closely and intervene only if there is a real need.
Early ergonomic field studies about controllers’ work activity
(Bisseret, 1971; Sperandio, 1971) indicated that controllers regulate
their work activity, using different strategies to control the work-
load. For example, controllers may reduce their workload by
regulating the amount of attention they give to some aircraft. This
regulation is based on the controller’s previous experience with the
aircraft/flight characteristics (e.g. aircraft that they believe have
conflict possibilities require more attention). These strategies
emerge within the controller’s daily work with the system and are
framed according to the system (man/technology/organization)
behavior. Therefore, to understand how and why this accident
emerged there are fundamental questions about the controllers’
activities that must be answered like, How frequently do the
‘‘abnormal’’ indications seen in this event occur in the controllers’
daily work? How much feedback do they have from supervisors?
What is the normal way they coordinate their activities with other
controllers? Unfortunately, we do not have ergonomic field studies
about Brazilian controllers’ activities to shed some light on these
questions.
The first action taken by a Brasilia ACC controller regarding flight
N600XL occurred at 16:28. It was a radio call requesting flight
N600XL’s crew to change to a new frequency to enable further
communication with Amazon ACC. This call occurred approxi-
mately 26 min after ATC ceased receiving transponder returns from
the flight. Flight N600XL was now flying almost at the limit of the
range of the sector 7 ATC radio transmitter, resulting in a situation
where although its radios were operating in a normal (proper) way,
it could not receive the ATC radio calls clearly.
The sector 7 Brasilia ACC controller also communicated to the
Amazon control center before the collision, to inform that flight
N600XL was proceeding northwest on UZ6. However, he did not
inform the Amazon controller that Brasilia ATC was not receiving
the flight’s transponder returns, or that the flight was no longer in
radar contact. This communication is presented in Table 6.
In this conversation, we see the same communication pattern
observed before, when two other controllers were talking about
flight N600XL’s flight plan. We note that the Brasilia controller only
informed about the communication frequency. He simply did not
mention any of the ‘‘abnormal’’ (at least in hindsight) indications
they had on the radarscope. At this moment, he (or his supervisor)
probably had already perceived the indication changes, but he did
not think that these indications were important enough to be
communicated to the fellow controller. This brings the important
question regarding how the system functions daily. If the situations
uncovered by this accident, like loss of radar contact, communica-
tions difficulties, and level variations due height-finding radar
Table 5
Frequencies used in the workplace 8. In bold the identified frequencies used by the
Legacy crew trying to communicate with Brasilia ACC. Two of them were not acti-
vated in the workplace 8 (source Câmara dos Deputados, 2007).
Sector Sector frequencies as presented
in the aeronautic chart of the
UZ6 airway
Frequencies activated
in the workplace 8
Frequencies not
activated in the
workplace 8
7
123.30–128.00–133.05–
135.90–121.50
135.90
123.30–128.00–
133.05
8
122.25–125.20–135.00–121.50 122.25–125.20
135.00
9
125.05–133.10–121.50
122.25–133.10
Source: CPI final report (Câmara dos Deputados, 2007).
Table 4
Major events and communications.
Time
Event
15:50:37
Legacy is leaving sector 5 of Brasılia ACC to enter in the sector 7. It
received instructions from sector 5 controller to use the radio
frequency of 125.05 in the new sector
15:51
Legacy entered in sector 7 of the Brasilia ACC
15:55:28
Planned flight level automatically (no controller action) changed
to 360 (370 ¼ 360)
16:01:53
Legacy transponder ceased to reply the ATC’s secondary radar
(370Z360 and þ)
16:17
Shift changeover in the workplace 8 (sectors 7, 8 and 9)
16:26:51 to
16:34:08
Brasilia ACC controller tries to communicate with Legacy – 6
attempts using the frequency of 125.05 Mhz
16:48:13 to
16:52:07
Legacy tries to communicate with Brasilia ACC controller – 12
attempts using several frequencies (2 attempts freq. 123.30, 1
attempt freq. 133.05 and 8 attempts with other non- identified
frequencies)
16:53:38
Brasilia ACC contact Legacy in the frequency135.09. Instructions to
Legacy contacts Amazon ACC in the frequency 123.32 or 126.45.
However, Legacy did not get the numbers.
16:56
The collision
Table 6
Communication between Brasilia and Amazon controller Dialog in Portuguese,
translated to English.
Time
Controller
Communication
16:53:30
Amazon ACC
Hi Brasilia
16:53:32
Brasilia ACC
November six zero zero x-ray lima. Do you have?
16:53:35
Amazon ACC
Yes, here.
16:53:37
Brasilia ACC
It is now entering in your area.
16:53:41
Amazon ACC
Yes, I have it. Yes, I have it.
16:53:45
Brasilia ACC
Good, three six zero. He is calling you there.
16:53:49
Amazon ACC
OK
16:53:50
Brasilia ACC
Bye
Source Câmara dos Deputados, 2007.
P.V. Rodrigues de Carvalho et al. / Applied Ergonomics 40 (2009) 325–340
338

inaccuracy are frequent enough in a way that ‘‘abnormal’’ indica-
tions were being considered ‘‘normal’’, then the ETTO principle and
associated heuristics (these things always happen, it is not impor-
tant to act now, the system is always changing symbols) function as
an important factor for the construction of cognitive strategies. In
this situation, we cannot attribute the cause of the accident to
a chain of human errors. Doing so, we will be blind to address the
real safety threats throughout the ATC system functioning.
5.5.4. The workplace in the Brasilia ACC during the event
All flight controllers who in some way acted in this event
(including those who worked during the clearance of the N600XL
flight in Sao Jose) are military controllers, sergeants of the Brazilian
Air Force, in a military activity, control of the air space, a task that is
attributed by Brazilian law to the Brazilian Air Force. The controllers
work in military workplaces, the Air space Control Centers (ACCs),
all under Department of Air space Control (DECEA) administration,
part of the structure of the Air Force Command (see Fig. 2).
In the ACCs, each workplace has 2 controllers, a main controller
and an assistant controller. A senior controller supervises each 2 or
3 workplaces. The investigation showed that at workplace 8 the
controllers that monitored sector 7 (including flight N600XL) did
not have an excessive number of aircraft under control (the
maximum number of aircraft is 12) during the period before
collision, and were therefore working in a low workload situation.
Despite the variation of the on screen indications, the investigation
concluded that there was no problem in the radar system software/
hardware, and the system functioned as it was designed to func-
tion. The investigation also emphasized that there were no wrong
indications or unexpected signals on the radarscope (Câmara dos
Deputados, 2007). Therefore, we can conclude that the systems
normally function as described above. To explain their actions the
controllers said in their testimonies (Câmara dos Deputados, 2007):
– Main controller: Before flight N600XL entered sector 7, he
communicated with the crew verifying that the aircraft was at
level 370 (the correct level at that moment). He did not antic-
ipate the need to change the level when the aircraft entered
sector 7. In the period from 18:55 (aircraft entered sector 7) up
to 19:17 (shift changeover), he perceived the loss of the
surveillance radar, but it did not alarm him. He said he was
satisfied with the information coming from the primary radar.
He informed his relief controller that flight N600XL was at level
360, because he knew about the inaccuracy of the primary
radar information and assumed that the aircraft was following
the flight plan that was displayed in the electronic strips.
– Assistant controller: He perceived that the N600XL did not
have complete information on the radar screen, and considered
that to be a normal situation. Even though uninformed of the
aircraft’s actual altitude, he coordinated with the Amazon ACC
controller the level of 360, based on the electronic flight strip
indication.
– Controller after shift changeover: He received flight N600XL at
level 360 and did not question the outgoing controller. He said
he had noticed the abnormal transponder functioning, and
tried 8 times to contact fight N600XL. However, he did not take
any action to avoid the conflict.
6. Conclusion
The accident described here opened a window onto the func-
tioning of the Brazilian air traffic system. As already noted in many
ergonomic field studies, in safety-critical systems operators’
cognitive strategies to maintain situation awareness are shaped by
the real conditions under which [they j operators] perform their
work, where resource limitations and uncertainty severely
constrain the choices and action opportunities. Cognitive task
analysis has been widely used (unfortunately not in Brazil) to
examine how air traffic controllers develop cognitive strategies to
manage their workload maintaining situation awareness. However,
most of the research focus is on how extreme traffic situations
influence the cognitive control strategies developed by the
controllers, rather than on how normal controllers working with
normal equipment in normal organizations shape their cognitive
strategies. In the events described here, we saw the influence of the
working constraints in shaping cognitive strategies that affect
system safety.
During the antecedents of the collision, there was no special air
traffic situation, no catastrophic equipment failure equipment, and
no trigger event as required in traditional accident models. This
accident emerges as a complex phenomenon within the normal
variability of the system functioning. This tragedy and many other
accidents in complex systems raise serious questions on how safety
is thought about in complex safety-critical systems. This accident
and accidents in other safety-critical systems have complex
patterns of emergence, where coincidences, unexpected links, and
resonance, substitute the old bullets such as equipment failure
probability, linear combinations of failures, human errors, and so
forth. Therefore, safety managers and engineers should review,
among many other things, how safety barriers should be used to be
effective in a defense-in-depth safety approach. The use of safety
barriers to stop the propagation of some trigger event cannot avoid
this type of accident simply because, as we have seen in this study,
there is nothing to be stopped. Almost didactically, we saw all the
barriers developed to avoid mid-air collisions melt down in a situ-
ation where everything functioned normally.
References
Barber, P., 1988. Applied Cognitive Psychology. Methuen, London.
Bisseret, A., 1971. An analysis of mental model processes involved in air traffic
control. Ergonomics 14, 565–570.
Brooker, P., 2006. Air traffic management accident risk. Part 1: the limits of realistic
modeling. Safety Science 44, 419–450.
Câmara dos Deputados, 2007. Relató rio final da comissa˜o parlamentar de inquérito
crise do sistema de tráfego aéreo. Câmara dos Deputados, Brasil.
Carvalho, P.V.R., 2005. Ergonomic field studies in a nuclear power plant control
room. Progress in Nuclear Energy 48 (1), 51–69.
Carvalho, P.V.R., Vidal, M.C.R., Carvalho, E.F., 2007. Nuclear power plant communi-
cations in normative and actual practice: a field study of control room opera-
tors’ communications. Human Factors in Ergonomics and Manufacturing 17 (1),
43–78.
Carvalho, P.V.R., Santos, I.J.A., Vidal, M.C., 2006. Safety implications of some cultural
and cognitive issues in nuclear power plant operation. Applied Ergonomics 37,
211–223.
Carvalho, P.V.R., Vidal, M.C., Santos, I.L., 2005. Nuclear power plant shift supervisor’s
decision-making during micro incidents. International Journal of Industrial
Ergonomics 35 (7), 619–644.
DECEA, 2006. Regras do ar e serviços de tráfego aéreo, ICA 100–12. Ministério da
Aeronáutica, Brasil (in Portuguese).
Dekker, S., 2006. Resilience engineering: chronicling the emergence of confused
consensus. In: Hollnagel, E., Woods, D.D., Leveson, N. (Eds.), Resilience Engi-
neering. Concepts and Precepts. Ashgate, Aldershot, UK.
Dailey, J., 1984. Characteristics of air traffic controller. In: Sells, S.B., Dailey, J.T.,
Pickerel, E.W. (Eds.), Selection of Air Traffic Controllers (No. FAA-AM-84-2).
Federal Aviation Administration Office of Aviation Medicine, Washington DC,
pp. 128–141.
Doherty, M.E., 1993. A laboratory scientist’s view of naturalistic decision making. In:
Klein, G.A., Orasanu, J., Calderwood, R., Zsambok, C.E. (Eds.), Decision Making in
Action: Models and Methods. Ablex Publishing Corp, Norwood, NJ.
EASA, 2005. Airworthiness Directive AD no: 2005-0021. European Aviation Safety
Agency, Germany.
Endsley, M.R., 1995. Toward a theory of situation awareness in dynamic systems.
Human Factors 37, 65–84.
Evans, J., 2004. Biases in deductive reasoning. In: R, Pohl (Ed.), Cognitive Illusions:
Handbook of Fallacies and Biases in Thinking, Judgment, and Memory.
Psychology Press, Hove, England.
Ferreira, R., 2006. Colisa˜o em Vôo, Presentation to the Brazilian Press in 29
September, 2006 (in Portuguese).
P.V. Rodrigues de Carvalho et al. / Applied Ergonomics 40 (2009) 325–340
339

Galison, P., 2000. An accident of history. In: Galison, P., Roland, A. (Eds.), Atmo-
spheric Flight in the 20th Century. Kluver Academic, Dordrecht, The Neder-
lands, pp. 3–44.
Holyoak, K., Simon, D., 1999. Bidirectional reasoning in decision making by
constraint satisfaction. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General 128, 3–31.
Hollnagel, E., 2004. Barriers and Accident Prevention. Ashgate, Aldershot, UK.
Hollnagel, E., 2006. Resilience – the challenge of unstable. In: Hollnagel, E.,
Woods, D.D., Leveson, N. (Eds.), Resilience Engineering. Concepts and Precepts.
Ashgate, Aldershot, UK.
Hollnagel, E., Woods, D.D., 2005. Joint Cognitive Systems: An Introduction to
Cognitive Systems Engineering. Taylor & Francis.
Irish Aviation Authority, 2005. Aeronautical Notice NR 0.52, issue 1, date 21 dec.
2005. Safety Regulation Division Irish Aviation Authority, Ireland.
Jones, D.G., Endsley, M.R., 1996. Sources of situational awareness errors in aviation.
Aviation, Space and Environmental Medicine 67, 507–512.
Kallus, K.W., Van Damme, D., Barbarino, M., 1999. Model of the Cognitive Aspects of
Air Traffic Control. European Air Traffic Management Programme Report.
Eurocontrol, Brussels.
Klein, G.A., 1993. A recognition-primed decision (RPD) model of rapid decision
making. In: Klein, G.A., Orasanu, J., Calderwood, R., Zsambok, C.E. (Eds.),
Decision Making in Action: Models and Methods. Ablex Publishing Corp,
Norwood, NJ.
Means, B., Mumaw, R.J., Roth, C., Schlager, M.S., Mc Williams, E., Gagné , E., 1988. ATC
Training Analysis Study: Design of the Next Generation of ATC Training System
(Rep. No. FAA/OPM 342–036). U.S. Department of Transportation – Federal
Aviation Administration, Washington DC.
Palmer, S.E., 1999. Vision science: Photons Phenomenology. MIT Press, Cambridge, MA.
Perrow, C., 1984. Normal Accidents. Basic Books, New York.
Quinn, S., Markovits, H., 1998. Conditional reasoning, causality, and the structure of
semantic memory: strength of association as a predictive factor for content
effects. Cognition 68, 93–101.
Rasmussen, J., 1983. Skills, rules and knowledge: signals, signs and symbols, and
other distinctions in human performance models. IEEE Transactions on
Systems, Man and Cybernetics 13, 257–266.
Rasmussen, J., Pejtersen, A., Goodstein, L., 1994. Cognitive Systems Engineering.
Wiley, New York.
Rasmussen, J., Svedung, I., 2000. Proactive Risk Management in a Dynamic Society.
Swedish Rescue Services Agency, Karlstad SW.
Reason, J., 1997. Managing the Risks of Organizational Accidents. Ashgate, London, UK.
Senado Federal, 2007. Relatório parcial dos trabalhos da Cpi ‘‘do apaga˜o aéreo’’.
Senado Federal, Brasılia (in Portuguese).
Snook, S.A., 2000. Friendly Fire: The Accidental Shootdown of U.S. Black Hawk
Helicopters Over Norther Iraq. Princeton University Press, U.K.
Sperandio, J.C., 1971. Variation of operator’s strategies and regulating effects on
workload. Ergonomics 14, 571–577.
Tversky, A., Kahneman, D., 1974. Judgment under uncertainty: heuristics and biases.
Science 185, 1124–1131.
Van ES, G.W.H., 2003. Review of Air Traffic Management-Related Accidents
Worldwide: 1980–2001, NLR-TP-2003-376. National Aerospace Laboratory NLR,
The Netherlands.
Woods, D.D., Cook, R.I., 2006. Incidents – markers of resilience or brittleness? In:
Hollnagel, E., Woods, D.D., Leveson, N. (Eds.), Resilience Engineering. Concepts
and Precepts. Ashgate, Aldershot, UK.
Yin, R.K., 1994. Case Study Research: Design and Methods. Sage, Thousand Oaks, CA.
P.V. Rodrigues de Carvalho et al. / Applied Ergonomics 40 (2009) 325–340
340